|
Lück, H. (2010). Group processes in scientific research (on example of Kurt Levin's topological group)
Journal |
Methodology and History of Psychology. 2010. Volume 5. Issue 3
|
Section |
Social Methodology of Psychology
|
Pages |
77-89
|
Type |
Scientific article
|
Title |
Group processes in scientific research (on example of Kurt Levin's topological group)
|
Authors |
Lück, Helmut
|
Abstract |
Based on the idea that scientific achievements are usually attributed to individuals, and social conditions of research and socio-psychological nature of scientific research is usually underestimated, the author shows that in modern science in general and in psychology in particular, scientific achievements are the result of group processes, and value of group processes in science is increasing. The theoretical basis for the study of group dynamics in science is social-psychological concept of coherent group (like-minded group) – small group whose members directly interact with each other, followed by the common goals and adhere to certain rules. Typical social and psychological processes in coherent group are leadership, cohesion, conformity, the formation of the group sense of "we-ness" and separation from other groups. The role of groups of scientists in the research of psychological issues considered by the example of three groups: the "Wednesday Psychological Society" by Sigmund Freud, "Cerebral cortex" by Oskar Pfungst, "Topological group" by Kurt Lewin (the latter is given special attention). Analyzing the activities of Kurt Lewin, the author stresses that Lewin was inclined to group discussion of scientific ideas: in the University of Berlin around him gathered group of students and doctoral candidates, and after emigrating of Lewin in the University of Iowa the similar group of students appeared. "Topological group", which from 1933 to 1964 (except for a few years during World War II) met every year for a few days, mainly during the Christmas holidays, played a special role in the development and dissemination of ideas of Lewin. Comparative analysis of the groups of scientists around Freud and Lewin shows that Lewin did not aspire to own scientific schools organization: colleagues and students of Lewin noted stimulating group atmosphere, informal and public nature of the group, the lack of strict liability to the ideas of Lewin. In its role as discussion platform, which was opened for discussion of ideas, including those different from topological theory, "Topological meetings" played significant role in the development of important research projects of modern psychology, including those related to the theoretical and applied areas of the teachings of Kurt Lewin.
|
Keywords |
- Kurt Lewin
- Sigmund Freud
- Oskar Pfungst
- social psychology of science
- role of group processes in scientific research
- styles of thinking in group
- processes of group dynamics in schools of psychology
- "Topological group"
- "Wednesday Psychological Society"
- "Cerebral cortex"
|
References |
- Abresch J., Lück H.E. Der Kluge Hans, Oskar Pfungst und die Hirnrinde // H. Gundlach (Hrsg.) Arbeiten zur Psychologiegeschichte. Göttingen, 1994. S. 83-94.
- Deutsch M., Krauss K.M. Theories in social psychology. N.Y., 1965.
- Fleck L. Entstehung und Entwicklung einer wissenschaftlichen Tatsache. Einführung in die Lehre vom Denkstil und Denkkollektiv. Frankfurt am Main, 1980. (Erstveröffentlichung: 1935.)
- Fölsing A. Der Mogelfaktor. Die Wissenschaftler und die Wahrheit. Hamburg; Zürich, 1984.
- Griffith B.C., Mullins N.C. Coherent social groups in scientific change // Science. 1972. Vol. 177. N 4053. (Deutsch: Kokärente soziale Gruppen im wissenschaftlichen Wandel // P. Weingart (Hrsg.) Wissenschaftssoziologie II. Determinanten wissenschaftlicher Entwicklung. Frankfurt, 1974. S. 223-238.)
- Guttmann A. Pfungst's Tätigkeit in der “Hirnrinde” // Oskar Pfungst zum Gedächtnis. Berlin, 1933. S. 21-24.
- Hagstrom W.O. The Scientific Community. N.Y., 1965. (Deutsche Teilübersetzung: Segmentierung als eine Form strukturellen Wandels in der Wissenschaft // P. Weingart (Hrsg.) Wissenschaftssoziologie I. Wissenschaftliche Entwicklung als sozialer Prozeß. Frankfurt am Main, 1972.)
- Heider F. The Life of a Psychologist. Kansas, 1983. (Deutsch: Das Leben eines Psychologen: eine Autobiographie. Bern, 1984.)
- Jones E. Sigmund Freud. Leben und Werk. Frankfurt am Main, 1969.
- Kuhn T.S. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, 1962. (Deutsch: Die Struktur wissenschaftlicher Revolutionen (mit Postskript). 3 Aufl. Frankfurt am Main, 1978.)
- Lück H.E. Zur Bedeutung der Gruppenprozesse für die Wissenschaftsentwicklung am Beispiel der Topology Group Kurt Lewins // Gestalt Theory. 1989. 11. S. 246-267.
- Marrow A.D. The practical theorist. The life and work of Kurt Lewin. N.Y., 1969. (Deutsch: Kurt Lewin – Leben und Werk. Stuttgart, 1977.)
- Masterman M. The nature of paradigm // I. Lakatos, A. Musgrave (Eds.) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge, 1970. P. 59-89. (Deutsch: Die Natur eines Paradigmas // I. Lakatos, A. Musgrave (Hrsg.) Kritik und Erkenntnisfortschritt. Braunschweig, 1974. S. 59-88.)
- Métraux A. Henri Piéron und die Wissenschaftspsychologie // Psychologische Rundschau. 1988. 39. S. 101.
- Nitzschke B. (Hrsg.) Freud und die akademische Psychologie. Beiträge zu einer historischen Kontroverse. München, 1989.
- Nunberg H., Federn E. Minutes of the Vienna Psychoanalytical Society. N.Y., 1962. (Deutsch: Protokolle der Wiener Psychoanalytischen Vereinigung. Frankfurt am Main, 1976.)
- Pfungst O. Das Pferd des Herrn von Osten. (Der kluge Hans). Ein Beitrag experimentellen Tier- und Menschen-Psychologie. Leipzig, 1907. (Zweitauflage unter dem Titel: Der kluge Hans – ein Beitrag zur nicht-verbalen Kommunikation. Frankfurt am Main, 1977.)
- Planck M. Wissenschaftliche Selbstbiographie. Leipzig, 1948.
- Price D.J. de S. Little science, big science. Columbia, 1963. (Deutsch: Little Science, Big Science. Von der Studierstube zur Großforschung. Frankfurt am Main, 1974.)
- Schmidt W. Struktur, Bedingungen und Funktionen von Paradigmen und Paradigmenwechsel. Eine wissenschafts-historisch-systematische Untersuchung der Theorie T.S. Kuhns am Beispiel der Empirischen Psychologie. Frankfurt am Main, 1981.
- Thomae H. Psychologiegeschichte in Coping-Perspektiven // A. Schorr, E.-G. Wehner (Hrsg.) Psychologiegeschichte heute. Göttingen, 1990. S. 1-11.
- Toulmin S. Human understanding: The collective use of concepts. Princeton, NJ, 1972. (Deutsch: Menschliches Erkennen. Frankfurt am Main, 1981.)
- Van Elteren M. Socio-psychology and the missing links within the historiography of psychological sciences. Paper presented to the Seventh Annual Conference of Cheiron-Europe, Budapest, 4-8 September, 1988.
- Archives of the History of American Psychology. Collection Donald K. Adams. Box 363. Folder Kurt Lewin.
- Frank J. 1967 // Archives of the History of American Psychology. The University of Akron. Kurt Lewin Collection. Box M 944. Folder 1.
- MacKinnon D.W. 1967 // Archives of the History of American Psychology, The University of Akron. Kurt Lewin Collection. Box M 944. Folder 1.
- Mead M. 1967 // Archives of the History of American Psychology. The University of Akron. Kurt Lewin Collection. Box M 944. Folder 1.
- Sears R.E. 19 Mai 1967 // Archive of the History of American Psychology. The University of Akron. Kurt Lewin Collection. Box M 944. Folder 1.
|
To cite this article |
Lück, H. (2010). Group processes in scientific research (on example of Kurt Levin's topological group). Methodology and History of Psychology, 5(3), 77-89.
|
|